![]() Healthcare Training Institute - Quality Education since 1979 CE for Psychologist, Social Worker, Counselor, & MFT!!
Section 21 Question 21 | Test | Table of Contents
Men are nine times more lethal than women, and adolescent boys are already dangerous. Can it be mere coincidence that this is the same time that escalating levels of male hormones flood the body and the nervous system? Is puberty the culminating event in a hormonally driven process of sexual differentiation that begins edging toward aggression even before birth? Studies in mice-a species in which prenatal hormonal manipulations are performed unwittingly by siblings-highlight the organizational role of sex hormones during brain development, an influence that will bias subsequent endocrine responsiveness. Female mouse fetuses positioned in utero between two males are exposed to more of their brothers' testosterone and less estrogen than females who develop next to a sister. As pups, these "2M" females have higher blood levels of testosterone; as adults, they boss other females and behave even more aggressively following testosterone injections. Males who spend the first weeks of their lives between two female fetuses, on the other hand, are less responsive to testosterone as adults. After birth, testosterone continues to oversee the process of sexual differentiation that began in the womb. Now hormones guide the first stages in the development of sexually specific behavior patterns-in males, for example, the propensity to attack other males rather than females. By the time mice and other rodents reach sexual maturity,
the weapons are in place. Exposure to testosterone during the critical period
of rapid brain development has titrated hormonal sensitivity and selected a battery
of sex-specific responses suitable for use in threatening circumstances. Puberty
finalizes mature patterns of social interaction: sexual advances toward females
and the stereotyped behavior patterns that characterize adult aggression toward
Unfamiliar males. An experiment by psychologists Renee Primus and Carol Kellogg, of the University of Rochester, demonstrates how sexual maturity brings a new awareness of external cues, linking testosterone, perception, and response. Female rats, paired with a stranger in a familiar cage or a new, previously unexplored enclosure, were equally sociable-sniffing, nudging, and grabbing-in both environments. Males, on the other hand, were much more sensitive to their surroundings. Beginning at puberty, male pairs placed in an unfamiliar cage tended to avoid one another, while pairs confined to a familiar environment interacted far more frequently, a difference prevented by castration at an early age. Testosterone, in other words, did not simply release or drive social behavior, but governed the ability to match key features of the environment with specific responses. A rat-or a human-who cannot make such distinctions is guaranteed to do the wrong thing at the wrong time. Testosterone levels in boys begin to surge at approximately age ten, rise rapidly over the next several years, and finally begin to plateau at about age fourteen, when aggressive behavior starts winding up. Coincidentally, this is also an age of rapid social change. Peer group rankings shift daily, with petty quarrels, athletic competitions, academic events, and girls offering ample opportunities to win or lose face. Conflicts with parents and other adults mark an urgent determination to muscle into the broader social structure, conflicts with higher stakes and a greater risk of defeat. Testosterone, now available in adult-strength doses, dutifully records each success or failure. But is it the rise in testosterone that causes violent behavior in adolescent boys? Data collected over the past decade by psychologist Elizabeth Susman (currently of Pennsylvania State University) and colleagues at NIMH and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development fail to support a relationship between antisocial behavior, including aggression, and elevated testosterone levels; in fact, these studies suggest just the opposite. Delinquent behavior, adjustment problems, and rebelliousness in adolescent boys aged ten to fourteen were associated with lower, not higher, testosterone levels. On the other hand, a Scandinavian study of fifty-eight Swedish boys between the ages of fifteen and seventeen (the last stage of puberty) did find a significant relationship between plasma testosterone levels and verbal, as well as physical, aggression. The most interesting feature of this study, however, was not what it said about the relationship between hormones and aggressive behavior, but what it said about the relationship between hormones and perception. Testosterone was most clearly linked to aggression when the violent behavior was a reaction to an overture perceived as a threat. Boys with higher levels of testosterone were more impatient and irritable, readily frustrated, and quick to take offense. The tendency to start fights, rather than fight back, was less strongly correlated with hormone levels. Testosterone, the report concluded, was less likely to drive aggression than it was to magnify a boy's "readiness to respond vigorously and assertively to threats and provocations." Threat perception involves neurotransmitters as well as hormones, in particular, the norepinephrine-containing neurons of the locus ceruleus that form the central branch of the global sympathetic response system. The power of this system lies in its flexibility, in its aptitude for tailoring the level of arousal to the level of risk posed by the environment. Each individual is born with an innate sensitivity to environmental stimuli-some more reactive, others less so. But the final working level of arousal is set by experience, by the nature of the interactions between the child and the outside world. This postnatal maturation process is reflected in the gradual development of the startle reflex. "From a developmental point of view, the startle reflex goes through some major changes in early childhood,"observes Robert Pynoos of the UCLA Neuropsychiatric Institute. Confront adults with a sudden high-pitched tone, and they jump, Pynoos explains. But warn them a fraction of a second earlier, and they're not so quick to startle. Two-year-olds don't habituate this way. They react even more intensely to the second stimulus, which, he notes, explains why his young daughter became more and more frightened during her first fireworks display. The tendency to overreact peaks between four and five years of age, then gradually declines.45 However, reaction times don't fall to adult values until about age eight, just before pubertal hormones kick in. Pynoos points out that the evolution of the startle reflex coincides with developmental changes in self-defense strategies. "If you put young children in a dangerous circumstance, they'll turn away from the danger and seek out protection. But about age eight, you start to hear the child fantasize about intervening actively: 'I wish I could have gotten the gun away from him.' It's a precursor to adolescence, when kids make real decisions about whether to intervene or not." The hormonal changes
of puberty therefore collide head-on with the first mature responses of the
brain's alarm system, in the volatile context of an unstable social structure
and frequent clashes with authority. But what happens when the alarm system has
already been overloaded, when an abusive parent, daily exposure to a violent,
dangerous neighborhood, or repeated harassment by a troubled sibling have made
it clear that the world is a dangerous place? Pynoos and his colleague, Edward
Ornitz, analyzed startle reflexes in school-age children suffering from PTSD and
found that their startle responses failed to habituate (Figure 6.3). "Instead
of a ten-year-old pattern, their responses look like a five-year-old pattern,"
he says. As a result, these children continue to overreact to environmental cues,
their catecholamine hazard-detection systems placed on alert by trauma. Instead
of relying on adults to protect them, as a five-year-old might, they've got a
ten-year-old's determination to solve their own problems. Tragically, the result
can be an adult decision to use a gun. Personal
Reflection Exercise #8 Update - Baidawi, S., Papalia, N., & Featherston, R. (2023). Gender Differences in the Maltreatment-Youth Offending Relationship: A Scoping Review. Trauma, violence & abuse, 24(2), 1140–1156. https://doi.org/10.1177/15248380211052106
QUESTION
21 |